Heredity and the ætiology of the neurosis 1896-001/1924.en
  • S.

    VIII

    HEREDITY AND THE ÆTIOLOGY OF THE
    NEUROSES:

    (1896)

    am addressing the pupils of J. M. Charcot espec-
    | to bring to their attention some objections

    to the theory concerning the ætiology of the neur-
    oses which was passed on to us by our master.

    The part ascribed to neurotic heredity in this
    theory is well known. It is the only true and indis-
    pensable cause of nervous disease; other etiological
    factors may aspire only to the title of precipitating
    causes. Thus the master himself and his pupils,
    Guinon, Gilles de la Tourette, Janet and others, have
    promulgated this doctrine for the major neurosis,
    hysteria; I believe that the same opinion is held
    in France and to some extent everywhere for the
    other neuroses, although it has not been laid down
    quite so solemnly and decidedly for conditions
    analogous to hysteria.

    For a long time I have entertained suspicions in
    this matter, but I had to wait to find corroborating
    facts in my daily medical experience. My objections
    are now of two kinds, arguments based upon facts
    and arguments of a more speculative order. I will
    begin with the former, arranging them according
    to the importance that I ascribe to them.

    I. (a) Sometimes diseases which are often enough
    foreign to the sphere of neuropathology and which
    do not necessarily depend upon disease of the nerv-

    1 First published in the Revue Neurologique, 1896, +. IV [Trans-
    lated by M. Meyer.]

  • S.

    1896 HEREDITY AND ÆTIOLOGY 139

    ous system have been considered nervous and
    indicative of neuropathic heredity. Thus, true facial
    neuralgias and many headaches have been considered
    nervous which are caused rather by post-infectious
    pathological changes and by suppuration in the nasal
    sinuses. I am convinced that these patients would
    profit if we were more often to leave the treatment
    of these conditions to rhinologists.

    (5) All nervous diseases found in the patient's
    family, without reference to their frequency or their
    gravity, have been accepted as justifying the accu-
    sation of nervous hereditary taint. Does not this
    point of view seem to imply a sharp distinction
    between families that are free from any nervous
    predisposition and families that are subject to it
    without limit or restriction? And do not the facts
    speak rather in favour of the opposite opinion,
    namely, that there are transitions and degrees of
    nervous predisposition and that no family escapes
    it entirely ?

    (c) Our opinion as to the ætiological rôle of hered-
    ity in nervous disease must assuredly be the result
    of an impartial statistical study and not of a petitio
    principii. Until this study has been made, the
    existence of acquired neuropathies should be con-
    sidered as probable as that of hereditary neuro-
    pathies. But if the existence of neuropathies acquired
    by persons not predisposed to them is possible, it
    can no longer be denied that some of the nervous
    diseases found among the relatives of our patients
    may have such an origin. They can then no longer
    be called upon as conclusive proofs of the hereditary
    predisposition that is imputed to the patient by
    virtue of his family history, for a retrospective
    diagnosis of the diseases of ancestors or of absent
    members of the family is only very rarely successful.

  • S.

    140 COLLECTED PAPERS VIII

    (d) Those who are adherents of the view of Four-
    nier and of Erb, concerning the part played by
    syphilis in the causation of tabes dorsalis and in
    progressive paralysis, have learned that powerful
    ætiological factors must be recognized the presence of
    which is indispensable in the pathogenesis of certain
    diseases that heredity alone could not cause. Charcot
    remained to the end, however, as I learned through
    a private letter from him, strictly opposed to Four-
    nier's theory, which, nevertheless, gains ground day
    by day.

    (e) There is no doubt that certain neuropathies.
    may develop in a man who was perfectly sound and
    of untainted family. One observes this every day
    in Beard's neurasthenia; if neurasthenia were limited
    to predisposed people, it would never have gained
    the importance and prevalence with which we are
    familiar.

    (f) In nervous pathology there is a so-called similar
    and a so-called dissimilar heredity. No fault will be
    found with the former concept; it is even very re-
    markable that in the affections that depend upon
    similar heredity (Thomsen's disease, Friedreich's
    disease, Huntington’s chorea, the myopathies, etc.),
    no trace of any accessory ætiological factor is ever
    found. But the much more important concept of
    dissimilar heredity has gaps that must be filled out
    to arrive at a satisfactory solution of the ætiological
    problems. They consist of the fact that members
    of the same family prove to be afflicted with the
    most varied forms of nervous disease, functional and
    organic, without our being able to discover a law
    that determines the substitution of one disease for
    another or the order in which they follow one another
    in successive generations. There are in these families,
    beside the affected members, persons who remain

  • S.

    1896 HEREDITY AND ÆTIOLOGY ェ ④ エ

    sound. The theory of dissimilar heredity tells us
    neither why one person carried the same hereditary
    taint without succumbing to it, nor why another
    selects, among the diseases that constitute the great
    family of neuropathies, one particular nervous affec-
    tion instead of another, hysteria instead of epilepsy,
    insanity, etc. Since there is no such thing as chance
    in the pathogenesis of nervous disease any more
    than elsewhere, it must be admitted that it is not
    heredity that controls the choice of a neuropathy
    developing in a member of a predisposed family,
    and that there is ground for suspecting the existence
    of other etiological factors of a less incomprehensible
    nature which deserve to rank as the specific ætiology
    of any such nervous disease. Without the existence
    of this special atiological factor heredity would have
    been powerless; it would have lent itself to the pro-
    duction of a different neuropathy if the specific
    «etiology in a given case had been replaced by any
    other.

    II. These specific, determining causes of neuropathy
    have been too little investigated, for the vision of
    physicians has been dazzled by the imposing pro-
    spect of the hereditary factor in etiology. Never-
    theless, they are well worth making the object of
    careful study; although their pathogenic power may
    be in general only auxiliary to that of heredity, great
    practical interest centres on the understanding of
    this specific etiology. It affords a point of attack
    for our therapeutic efforts, while hereditary predis-
    position, predetermined for the patient from birth,
    frustrates all our efforts by the strength of its position.

    I have been engaged for years in a study of the
    ætiology of the major neuroses (functional nervous
    states analogous to hysteria), and in what follows
    I shall report the result of these studies. In order

  • S.

    142 COLLECTED PAPERS VIII

    to avoid all possible misunderstanding, I shall begin
    by making two observations on the nosography of
    the neuroses and on their atiology in general.

    I had to start my work with an innovation in noso-
    graphy. I have found reason to place the obsessional
    neurosis upon the same footing as hysteria, as an
    independent and autonomous disease, although the
    majority of authors classify obsessions among the
    syndromes comprising mental degeneration or else
    confound them with neurasthenia. I have learned,
    by examining their mental mechanism, that these
    disorders are much more closely allied to hysteria
    than one would have believed.

    Hysteria and the obsessional neurosis form the
    first group of the major neuroses that I studied.
    The second contains Beard’s neurasthenia, which I
    split up into two functional conditions differentiated
    both ætiologically and symptomatologically: neur-
    asthenia proper and anxiety-neurosis—a term, by
    the way, which does not altogether please me. I
    have given detailed reasons for this differentiation,
    which I consider necessary, in a paper published in
    1895.

    As for the ætiology of the neuroses, I think we
    should recognize that the various ætiological factors,
    differing in their importance and in the way in which
    they are related to the effect they produce, may be
    arranged theoretically in three classes: (1) Conditions
    that are indispensable for the production of the
    disease in question, but which are of a universal
    nature and occur as well in the ætiology of many
    other diseases. (2) Concurrent causes that share with
    the conditions in group (I) the characteristic of
    entering into the causation of other diseases as well
    as into that of the disease in question, but which

    1 See No. V of this volume, above, p. 76.

  • S.

    1896 HEREDITY AND ETIOLOGY 143

    are not indispensable for the production of the latter
    disease. (3) Specific causes just as indispensable as
    the first conditions but appearing only in the ætio-
    logy of the disease for which they are specific.

    Now, in the pathogenesis of the major neuroses
    heredity plays the part of a condition, potent in all
    cases and even indispensable in the majority of
    them. It cannot do without the assistance of the
    specific causes; but the importance of hereditary
    predisposition is demonstrated by the fact that
    the same specific causes operating on a sound person
    would produce no manifest pathological effect, while
    its presence in a predisposed person will precipitate
    a neurosis, the development of which will in inten-
    sity and extent be proportional to the degree of
    hereditary predisposition.

    The action of heredity is comparable to that of
    a multiplier in an electric circuit, which increases the
    visible deviation of the needle but which cannot
    determine its direction.

    There is another point to be noted in the relations
    between the hereditary condition and the specific
    causes of the neuroses. Experience shows, as might
    have been anticipated, that among the problems
    of etiology that of the quantitative relationship of
    the ætiological factors to one another should not
    be neglected. But one would not have guessed the
    fact which seems to follow from my observations,
    that heredity and the specific causes may replace
    one another quantitatively, that the same patho-
    logical effect will be produced by the co-existence
    of a very grave specific ætiology and a moderate
    degree of predisposition as by that of a severe neuro-
    pathic heredity with a slight specific factor. So that it is
    merely a quite possible extreme in this series when
    one finds cases of neurosis in which a tangible degree

  • S.

    144 COLLECTED PAPERS VIII

    of hereditary predisposition is looked for in vain,
    provided that this deficiency is compensated for by
    a powerful specific factor.

    As concurrent or accessory causes all those ‘ordinary’
    factors found elsewhere may be mentioned: mental
    excitement, physical exhaustion, acute illness, in-
    toxication, traumatic accidents, intellectual over-
    work, etc. I would maintain that none of them, not
    even the last, enters regularly or necessarily into
    the ætiology of neuroses, well knowing that the
    enunciation of this opinion is in direct opposition
    to a theory regarded as universal and incontestable.
    Since Beard declared neurasthenia to be the pro-
    duct of our modern civilization he has found only
    believers; but it is impossible for me to accept this
    opinion. A painstaking study of the neuroses has
    taught me that the specific etiology of the neuroses
    has escaped Beard’s attention.

    I do not wish to underestimate the etiological
    importance of these every-day factors. They are
    very varied, of frequent occurrence, and most often
    blamed by the patient; they are more manifest than
    the specific causes, which are either concealed or
    not understood. They often fulfil the function of
    precipitating causes bringing the hitherto latent
    neurosis to the surface; and a practical interest
    attaches to them, since consideration of these ordin-
    ary causes may furnish a basis for a therapy that
    does not aim at a radical cure, but contents itself
    with reducing the disease to its previous latent state.

    But a constant and intimate relation between a
    given ordinary cause and a given nervous affection
    cannot be established; mental excitement, for ex-
    ample, is found as often in the ætiology of hysteria,
    of obsessions, of neurasthenia, as in that of epilepsy,
    Parkinson’s disease, diabetes, and numerous others.

  • S.

    1896 HEREDITY AND ÆTIOLOGY 145

    The ordinary concurrent causes can also replace the
    specific ætiology quantitatively, but can never com-
    pletely supplant it. There are numbers of cases in
    which hereditary predisposition and the specific factor
    constitute the entire ætiology, the ordinary causes
    being absent. In other cases the indispensable ætio-
    logical factors are not strong enough in themselves
    to precipitate the neurosis and a state of apparent
    good health may be maintained for a long time, a
    state that is really one of neurotic predisposition.
    It requires only the added effect of an ordinary
    cause to make the neurosis become manifest. It
    must be noted, however, that in such circumstances
    the nature of the supervening ordinary factor,
    whether it be excitement, trauma, infectious disease,
    or any other, is entirely a matter of indifference;
    the pathological result does not vary in accordance
    with it, and the nature of the neurosis will always
    be determined by the pre-existing specific cause.

    What then are the specific causes that produce
    neuroses? Is there only one or are there several?
    And can a constant ætiological relationship be
    established between a given cause and a given neur-
    osis so that each major neurosis may be referred to
    a particular etiology ?

    Supported by a painstaking investigation of the
    facts, I wish to affirm that this hypothesis agrees
    well with the truth, that each of the major neuroses
    mentioned has as its immediate cause a special
    disturbance of the nervous economy, and that these
    pathological functional changes betray, as their com-
    mon source, the sexual life of the person concerned,
    either a disturbance of his present sexual life or import-
    ant events in his past life.

    This is, strictly speaking, not a new, unheard-of
    proposition. It has always been admitted that sexual

    10

  • S.

    146 COLLECTED PAPERS VIII

    disturbances were among the causes of nervousness,
    but they were grouped together on a level with other
    precipitating causes as subordinate to heredity; their
    ætiological importance was restricted to a limited
    number of the cases studied. Physicians had even
    acquired the habit of not looking for them if the
    patient did not himself accuse them. The distinctive
    characteristic of my point of view lies in my raising
    the sexual factors to the rank of specific causes, in
    my recognizing their influence in all cases of neurosis,
    and finally in my, finding a constant parallelism,
    proof of a special ætiological relationship, between
    the nature of the sexual factor and the kind of neur-
    osis.

    I am quite sure that this theory will provoke a
    storm of contradiction from my fellow physicians.
    But this is not a fitting occasion to present the data
    and observations that have imposed my conviction
    on me, nor to explain the strict meaning of the
    somewhat vague term ‘disturbances of nervous econ-
    omy’. This will be done, I hope as thoroughly as
    possible, in a work that I am preparing on the sub-
    ject. In this paper I shall confine myself to the
    presentation of my results.

    Pure neurasthenia, which after it has been differ-
    entiated from anxiety-neurosis presents a monoton-
    ous clinical picture (exhaustion, sense of pressure
    on the head, flatulent dyspepsia, constipation, spinal
    paræsthesias, sexual weakness, etc.), admits of only
    two specific ætiological factors, excessive onanism
    and spontaneous emissions.

    The prolonged and intense effect of this pernicious
    form of sexual satisfaction alone suffices to produce
    neurasthenia, or else it stamps the subject with the
    specific neurasthenic habitus which later is made
    manifest under the influence of a chance accessory

  • S.

    1896 HEREDITY AND ETIOLOGY I47

    cause. I have also met persons who showed the
    signs of a neurasthenic constitution, but in whom I
    did not succeed in demonstrating the ætiology just
    mentioned; I was able at least to prove, however,
    that in these patients the sexual function had never
    developed to a normal degree. They seemed to be
    endowed by heredity with a sexual organization
    analogous to that which arises in the neurasthenic
    as a result of onanism.

    The anxiety-neurosis, the clinical picture of which
    is much richer (irritability, states of anxious ex-
    pectation, phobias, more or less severe panics, fear,
    vertigo, trembling, sweats, congestion, dyspnæa,
    tachycardia, etc.; chronic diarrhæa, chronic loco-
    motor vertigo, hyperæsthesia, insomnia, etc.)! is
    easily shown to be the specific result of various
    disturbances of sexual life, all having a common
    trait. Forced abstinence, frustrated sexual excite-
    ment (not gratified by sexual intercourse), incomplete
    or interrupted coitus (not attaining gratification),
    sexual efforts that exceed the psychical capacity
    of the person, etc., all these factors, all too common
    in modern life, seem to unite in disturbing the equi-
    librium of psychical and somatic functions in sexual
    activity, and in hindering the psychical co-operation
    necessary to relieve the nervous economy from
    sexual tension.

    These considerations, which contain perhaps the
    nucleus of a theoretical explanation of the functional
    mechanism of the neurosis in question, arouse the
    suspicion that a complete and really scientific demon-
    stration of the matter is not at the moment possible,
    and that the problem of the physiology of the sexual
    life will first have to be approached from a new angle.

    1 For the symptomatology and ætiology of the anxiety-neurosis
    see my paper cited above, No. V of this volume, p. 76.

    10*

  • S.

    på 48 COLLECTED PAPERS VIII

    I will conclude by stating that the presence of
    hereditary predisposition is not indispensable in the
    pathogenesis of neurasthenia and of the anxiety-
    neurosis. This is the result of daily observation; if
    the hereditary factor is present, however, the develop-
    ment of the neurosis will be strongly influenced
    thereby.

    In the second class of major neuroses, hysteria and
    the obsessional neurosis, the solution of the ætio-
    logical problem is surprisingly simple and uniform.
    I owe my conclusions to the use of the new psycho-
    analytic method, the probing procedure of J. Breuer,
    a method that is somewhat subtle but irreplaceable,
    so fruitful has it proved to be in explaining obscure
    unconscious mental processes. By means of this
    method, which cannot be explained here,” hysterical
    symptoms are traced to their origin, which invariably
    proves to be an experience in the person’s sexual
    life well adapted to produce a painful emotional
    reaction. Going back into the patient’s life step
    by step, guided always by the structural connection
    between symptoms, memories, and associations, I
    finally came to the starting-point of the pathological
    process; and I had to realize that the same factor
    was at the bottom of all the cases subjected to
    analysis, namely, the effect of an agent that must
    be accepted as the specific cause of hysteria.

    It is indeed a memory connected with the person’s
    sexual life, but one that presents two extremely
    important features. The event, the unconscious
    image of which the patient has retained, is a premat-
    ure sexual experience with actual stimulation of the
    genitalia, the result of sexual abuse practised by

    1 [The first use of this term by the author to designate his tech-
    nique.—Ed.]
    ? See J. Breuer and Sigmund Freud. Studien über Hysterie. 1895.

  • S.

    1896 HEREDITY AND ETIOLOGY 149

    another person, and the period of life in which this
    fateful event occurs is early childhood, up to the
    age of eight to ten, before the child has attained
    sexual maturity.

    A passive sexual experience before puberty: this is
    the specific etiology of hysteria.

    I will at once supplement these conclusions by
    some detailed facts and some explanatory remarks,
    so as to combat the doubt that I anticipate. I have
    been able to analyse thirteen cases of hysteria com-
    pletely. Three of this number were true combinations
    of hysteria with an obsessional neurosis (I do not
    say hysteria with obsessions). The experience men-
    tioned above was not lacking in a single case; it was
    present either as a brutal attempt committed by
    an adult or as a less sudden and less repugnant
    seduction, having however the same result. In seven
    cases out of the thirteen we were dealing with a
    liaison between children, sexual relations between
    a little girl and a boy slightly older, generally her
    brother who had himself been the victim of an
    earlier seduction. These liaisons were sometimes
    continued for years, up to puberty, the boy repeating
    upon the little girl without alteration those practices
    that he had himself experienced at the hands of a
    servant or governess; because of this origin they
    were often of a disgusting kind. In some cases there
    had been both assaults and an infantile liaison or
    repeated brutal abuse.

    The date of the premature experience was variable:
    in two cases it went back to the second year (?) of
    the child; in my observations the age of predilection was
    the fourth or fifth year. It may be a matter of chance,
    but I have the impression from them that a passive
    sexual experience occurring after the age of eight or
    ten can no longer be the foundation of a neurosis.

  • S.

    150 COLLECTED PAPERS VIII

    How can one be convinced of the truth of these
    confessions made in analysis and said to be memories
    preserved since early childhood, and how can we
    protect ourselves against the inclination to fabricate
    and the facility for invention ascribed to hysterics?
    I would charge myself with blameworthy credulity
    if I did not offer more convincing proofs. But the
    fact is that patients never relate these histories
    spontaneously, and never suddenly offer, in the
    course of the treatment, the complete recollection
    of such a scene to the physician. The mental image
    of the premature sexual experience is recalled only
    when most energetic pressure is exerted by the
    analytic procedure, against strong resistance; so that
    the recollection has to be extracted bit by bit from
    the patients, and while it comes back into conscious-
    ness they fall prey to emotions difficult to simulate.

    If we remain uninfluenced by the patient’s be-
    haviour and are able to follow impartially, in detail,
    the psycho-analysis of a case of hysteria, we are
    finally convinced ourselves.

    The premature experience in question has left a
    permanent imprint upon the history of the case,
    being represented in it by a mass of symptoms and
    peculiar features that permit of no other explanation;
    the delicate but firm inter-relationship of the struc-
    tural elements of the neurosis compel us to accept
    this view; the therapeutic result is retarded if we
    do not go so deep as this; no other choice exists then
    except that of rejecting or believing the whole matter.

    Is it to be believed that a premature sexual ex-
    perience of this kind, undergone by a person whose
    sex is scarcely differentiated, may become the
    starting-point of a permanent mental abnormality
    like hysteria? And further, how would such a view
    harmonize with our present conception of the psych-

  • S.

    1896 HEREDITY AND ETIOLOGY I5I

    ical mechanism of that neurosis? A satisfactory
    reply can be given to the first question: it is just
    because the subject is a child that premature sexual
    stimulation produces little or no effect at the time,
    but a mental impression of it is retained. Later, at
    puberty, when the sexual organs have developed a
    degree of activity immeasurably greater than that
    of childhood, this unconscious mental impression
    is somehow or other reawakened. Owing to the
    changes produced by puberty the memory will
    exercise a power which was entirely lacking when
    the experience itself took place; the memory will
    produce the same result as if it were an actual event.
    We have, so to speak, the subsequent effect of a sexual
    trauma.

    As far as I know, the reawakening of a sexual
    recollection after puberty, the event itself having
    occurred at a time antedating this period, affords
    the only psychological conjunction in which the
    effect of a memory surpasses that of the event itself.
    But it is an abnormal constellation, which attacks
    the mind at a weak point and necessarily produces
    a pathological result.

    I believe that this inverse relation between the
    psychical effect of the recollection and that of the
    event contains the reason why the memory has
    remained unconscious.

    We thus approach a very complex psychical
    problem, one which, however, if duly appreciated
    promises to throw a flood of light some day upon
    the most intricate problems of mental life.

    The ideas set forth here, based upon the results
    of psycho-analysis, namely, that the memory of a
    premature sexual experience is always found as the
    specific cause of hysteria, do not agree with Janet's
    psychological theory of the neurosis, nor with any

  • S.

    152 COLLECTED PAPERS VIII

    other; but they harmonize perfectly with my own
    hypotheses, described elsewhere, on the 'defence
    neuroses’.

    All events occurring after puberty to which an
    influence on the development of hysteria and the
    formation of its symptoms must be ascribed are
    only concurrent causes, “agents provocateurs' in the
    words of Charcot, to whom neuropathic heredity
    occupied the position that I claim for premature
    sexual experiences. These accessory factors are not
    subject to the strict rules that govern the specific
    causes; analysis demonstrates beyond question that
    they produce a pathological effect in hysteria only
    by virtue of their ability to awaken the unconscious
    mental impression of the infantile experience. It is
    also because of their connection with the original
    pathogenic impression and by its magnetism, so to
    speak, that the memory of them becomes unconscious
    in turn, and is able to assist the growth of a mental
    process withdrawn from the influence of conscious
    processes.

    The obsessional neurosis arises from a specific
    cause closely analogous to that of hysteria. A pre-
    mature sexual experience, which has occurred before
    puberty and the memory of which becomes active
    during or after that period, is also found here and the
    same observations and arguments made in reference
    to hysteria apply to cases of this neurosis (six cases,
    three pure forms). There is only one difference that
    seems of capital importance. At the base of the
    ætiology of hysteria we found a passive sexual
    experience undergone with indifference or with a
    slight degree of disgust or fright. In the obsessional
    neurosis we are concerned, on the other hand, with
    an experience that was pleasurable, with a sexual
    aggression inspired by desire (in the case of the

  • S.

    1896 HEREDITY AND ETIOLOGY 153

    boy) or with a pleasurable participation in sexual
    acts (in the case of the little girl). The obsessive ideas,
    whose inner meaning is made recognizable by ana-
    lysis, reduced, so to speak, to their simplest form,
    are nothing but reproaches that the patient makes
    to himself because of that premature sexual pleasure,
    but reproaches disguised by an unconscious psychical
    work of transformation and substitution.

    The very fact that this sexual aggression occurs
    at such a tender age seems to betray the effect of
    a previous seduction, which would result in pre-
    cocity of sexual desire. Analysis has confirmed this
    suspicion in the cases I have treated. In this way
    an interesting fact always present in these obsessional
    cases is explained, namely, the regular way in which
    the clinical picture is complicated by a certain
    number of purely hysterical symptoms.

    The importance of the active sexual attitude as
    a cause of obsessions, and that of the passive attitude
    in the pathogenesis of hysteria, seems to give the
    reason of hysteria’s close association with the female
    sex and of the preference of the male for the obsession-
    al neurosis. One sometimes comes across a pair of
    neurotics who had a liaison in early childhood, the
    man suffering from obsessions, the woman from
    hysteria; if we are dealing with brother and sister
    we may mistake for the result of heredity what is
    really derived from premature sexual experience.

    There are, unquestionably, pure and isolated cases
    of hysteria and obsessional neurosis, independent of
    neurasthenia and anxiety-neurosis, but they are not
    the rule. More frequently, the psychoneurosis ap-
    pears as accessory to the neurasthenic neuroses,
    having been provoked by the latter and following
    their decline. This happens because the specific
    causes of the latter, current disturbances of the

  • S.

    154 COLLECTED PAPERS VIII

    sexual life, act at the same time as accessory causes
    of the psychoneuroses, the specific cause of which,
    the memory of an early sexual experience, they re-
    awaken and revive.

    As for neurotic heredity, I am far from being able
    to estimate accurately its influence in the etiology
    of the neuroses. I admit that its presence is indis-
    pensable in serious cases; I doubt whether it is a
    sine qua non in mild cases, but I am convinced that
    neurotic heredity alone cannot cause a psycho-
    neurosis if the specific etiology of the latter, premat-
    ure sexual stimulation, be lacking. I even believe
    that the question which of the two neuroses, hysteria
    or obsessional neurosis, will develop in a given case
    is determined not by heredity but by the particular
    nature of the early sexual experience in childhood.